Ontology : a branch of philosophy
dealing with the nature of reality
All social scientists have ideas
about the nature and characteristics of whatever it is they are studying. These
are the researchers' ontological assumptions; these assumptions refer to what
researchers think exists and is real. These ideas are seldom consciously questioned
by researchers because they appear so obvious (Dooley 1990:6; Wilson 1983:2,10).
Epistemology: a branch of philosophy
dealing with the nature of knowledge
As social scientists we do not only
make assumptions about the nature of the subject matter studied. We also need
to consider the nature of our knowledge. When we do this, we focus on the
structure or format of our knowledge rather than on its content.
Epistemological questions deal with
how we can know and explain something. This means we have to decide what types
of statements about social reality are permissible.
In other words, we have to decide
what qualifies as being social scientific knowledge (Mason 1996:13; Wilson
1983:2).
Methodology:
the rules
and procedures of research work
Once we have decided how we can get
to know social reality, we can then think about the best way to do this.
Goldenberg (1992:18) argues that methodological principles in the social
sciences ensure that we can defend our
findings. Methodological principles are those guidelines that researchers
agree on and that they rely on to give us acceptable research practices.
Methodological principles enable researchers to attain knowledge by providing
researchers with the necessary techniques or tools (Babbie 1995:18; Denzin
1989:4; Mason 1996:35).
We cannot be rigid about the actual
distinctions between ontology, epistemology and methodology, since all three
are interlinked (Williams & May 1996:88).
Approaches
The enormous diversity in research is
an inevitable consequence of the complexity of social reality. Nevertheless,
this diversity can become overwhelming and disorienting.
By considering researchers'
ontological, epistemological and methodological assumptions, we can identify
different approaches to social science research. What we are trying to do here
is identify the assumptions that guide social scientists' research. By identifying
researchers' basic assumptions, we can come to a better understanding of social
research and get a better idea of the key issues in social research. Knowing
and understanding different approaches allows us to compare these approaches systematically.
There are three dominant approaches to
social science: (1) the positivist
approach (2) interpretive approach and (3) critical approach (Ashley &
Orenstein 1990:38; Neuman 1997:62; Sarantakos 1998:32; Stevens in Sapsford et
al 1998:79±80).
The researchers who follow these
different approaches share basic assumptions on how social reality should be
viewed, what problems social scientists should address, how they can know
social reality and what the purpose of research is.
The
positivist approach
Positivism: a systematic way of doing
research that emphasises the importance of observable facts relationships:
Positivists believe that social
reality can be discovered. They argue that we can perceive social reality
through our senses since it exists ``out there'', independent of the ``knower''.
According to positivists, the behaviour of human beings is determined by external
influences that produce particular effects under certain conditions. People react
predictably to their environment because they are rational individuals. Once positivists
have identified certain regularities, they use these regularities to explain
social events and relationships. Such regularities are called social laws.
According to positivists, social reality reflects certain patterns and the
behaviour of human beings is, to an extent, a product of these patterns.
However, because there are a number of factors at play in the environment,
positivists emphasise that social laws should be treated as probabilities
rather than certainties (May 1993:5; Sarantakos 1998:36).
Value-freedom:
researchers'
personal values do not influence the collection of data (research)
The emphasis positivists place on
discovering social regularities suggests that they believe that social sciences
should be studied in the same way as natural sciences.
Positivists argue that, since social
phenomena exist in their own right, social phenomena are open to outside
empirical observation.
For positivists, all knowledge is based
on facts. Facts are empirically established by the senses. Positivists claim it
is crucial that researchers approach social reality in a neutral, value-free,
detached and systematic way.
Methodological tools are developed to
collect evidence that is observable and hence measurable. Standardised
procedures are followed to study particular events and learn about their
interconnections. These procedures furthermore ensure a detached approach that
will represent social reality accurately and free from bias. Once these
regularities have been established by checking them against the facts, the
knowledge gained can be used to predict occurrences and control events (Sarantakos
1998:37,38,61; Wilson 1983:11±12).
Positivists regard knowledge to be
cumulative. This means that all current knowledge of a topic can be used when
studying this topic. Once research is completed, new knowledge is added.
Positivists want to give social science more powers of explanation.
They believe it is right for us to
use our knowledge of causal social laws to help society progress. For instance,
once we identify the causes of group conflict, we can prevent such conflict
from occurring. For example, let us assume that one of the causes of group
conflict is stereotyping of an out group. In this case, we can at least defuse
the possibility of conflict by taking steps to prevent stereotyping (O'Brien in
Gilbert 1993:7;
The
interpretive approach
interpretivism: an approach to social
science that emphasises the importance of insiders'
viewpoints to understanding social realities. Interpretivists argue that social
reality is inherently meaningful. People have the ability to interpret a
situation and decide how to act in response to this situation. By consciously
participating in a situation, they attribute meaning to that situation.
Meaning is constructed through human
beings interacting with each other and playing a central role in defining a
situation to make sense of it. These meanings are generated in a social process
and often shared intersubjectively. Patterns and regularities in behaviour emerge
from the social conventions established by purposefully interacting human beings.
These patterns emerge from an intersubjective understanding of the meaningfulness
of a situation (Sarantakos 1998:36±37; Wilson 1983:9,114±116). In this respect
these patterns are viewed as implicative rather than strictly causal.
Interpretivists argue that the
purpose of research is to make social reality intelligible and reveal its
inherent meaningfulness. Meaningful actions need to be understood from within.
This requires studying how social reality is experienced, interpreted and understood.
In this respect interpretivists claim that there is no external social reality.
Social reality is created through
conscious actions of human beings. Interpretive social scientists therefore
argue that there is no basis for using the same methods as the natural
sciences. They insist that social reality differs fundamentally from natural
reality and to explain social actions we need to first understand the
meaningfulness of social reality (Denzin 1989:5; Mason 1996:47).
But how do interpretivists access the
meaningfulness of social reality? They argue that researchers have to be
sensitive to the social context in which meaning is produced.
Social reality is approached from the
perspective of the human beings who actively construct this reality. One useful
strategy is to pay attention to common sense because this provides insight into
human beings' own understanding of their situation.
Paying attention to human beings'
understanding of their own situation enables researchers to see how people
construct and understand these situations intuitively.
Value-freedom, neutrality or
detachment is of little use to the interpretive researcher since there is no
external reality independent of the particular context in which meaning is
attributed. Interpretivists see values as an integral part of social reality
and emphasise that they should be acknowledged as such (Bailey 1996:28; Mason 1996:4,6;
Ragin 1994:43±44; Sarantakos 1998:38).
They argue that research into social
reality is justified by its ability to demonstrate the meaningfulness of social
interaction in a particular context. Successful research must provide outsiders
with adequate understanding of the situation so that they can communicate with
insiders (Ashley & Orenstein 1990:41; Wilson 1983:120±121).
The critical
approach: an
approach to social science that emphasises the need to uncover hidden processes
and structures within society.
Critical social scientists argue that
social reality is multi-layered. Although social reality presents us with a
facade there are more dimensions to social reality than initially appear to us.
In this respect it is crucial to move
from the surface structures (the way social reality presents itself) to the
underlying mechanisms by which social reality is maintained. We need to
penetrate the layers of social reality and uncover underlying relationships
that determine its real characteristics to truly understand its nature.
For instance, processes and practices
designed to sustain the current state of affairs in a society often mask the
mechanisms that actually result in inequality and exploitation within a
society. Relationships of authority are reinforced through interaction. The dominant
discourse presents a view of reality constructed by the powerful. This discourse
serves the interests of the powerful by manipulating and conditioning others to
accept it as correct. A false consciousness is created when people accept the situation
as being natural and therefore unconsciously reproduce the relationships (social
structure) that govern them. It is only when the illusion of the social reality
as it presents itself is exposed and the underlying tensions and contradictions
become apparent that the full potential of human creativity and agency can be
unleashed. The awareness created by exposing the false consciousness enables
people to reflect on how they are both products and creators of social reality.
Social reality therefore becomes a
human construction with more than one possibility. Once this awareness is created,
the transformation of social reality is possible. It should be noted that
although human beings have the potential to change social reality through their
actions, the structures of social reality enables or constrains these actions
(Ashley & Orenstein 1990:41±42; Sarantakos 1998:36±37,60±61; Sayer 1992:40;
Wilson 1983:9,168,175)
How then can we get to know this
multi-layered social reality? Critical social scientists argue that positivist
social scientists' emphasis on discovering objective facts that are observable
and measurable is misguided because social reality cannot be taken only at its
face value. Critical social scientists claim that observable surface structures
seldom coincide with reality. This is why critical social scientists rely on
theory. Their theory provides models or analogies that reveal the hidden
structures that determine the key characteristics of social reality. By using
the logic and reasoning of these abstract frameworks (theory) researchers can
work out the implications of the underlying, hidden dynamics of social reality.
They can then predict how the consequences of these hidden dynamics will reveal
themselves in observable surface structures (ie the ``facade'' we referred to
earlier on). According to critical social scientists, empirically observed
patterns (ie the patterns we can see) are the evidence that points to the underlying
mechanisms. When observations confirm the predictions made by theory, researchers'
are more confident that these unobservable structures do, in fact, exist.
Note that critical social scientists
do not reject facts, but argue that the truth of social reality goes beyond
empirical facts.
Critical social scientists also argue
that the interpretive approach perpetuates the myths of social reality by
reducing it to common-sense interpretation and understanding. This stance is
taken because critical social scientists claim that common-sense understandings
are contaminated by a false consciousness. They do not reject attempts to
understand common-sense ideas, but they warn that these are only partial and incomplete.
For critical social scientists understanding the subjective experiences of human
beings cannot be the end goal of social science. Instead, science should transcend
barriers by examining the unexamined and critically reflecting on it (Richards 1989:3,
126; Sayer 1992:4,39±40; Wilson 1983:166,173±174).
Critical social sciences assume that
a critical and reflective researcher will actively engage with the subject matter.
Objectivity is not a goal because researchers are morally committed to
challenging inequalities and domination. In order to emancipate human beings
from the ideology that sustains their false consciousness they need to be made
aware of the underlying mechanisms that structure their daily lives. This awareness
of the real nature of social reality will empower human beings to work towards
meaningful social change and transformation. Theory serves as a guide by suggesting
possible progressive alternatives. By exposing the current underlying mechanisms
that need to be challenged, theory provides a critical and activist agenda.
The quantitative approach
Mouton and
Marais (1989:157) define ``quantitative approach'' as
follows:
... the
approach used by researchers in the social sciences that is more formalised in
nature as well as explicitly controlled, with a more carefully defined scope,
and that is relatively close to the approach used by researchers in the natural
sciences (translation).
This
approach aims at examining the generally accepted explanations of phenomena, and
is therefore more structured and controlled in nature. The scope of this
type of approach is larger and more universal in nature, and can also be
defined accurately. An example here would be research into the nature and scope
of gang activities in, say, South African prisons. For the research to be valid
and reliable, use is made of specific scientific methods and techniques (the
survey method [such as questionnaires], random sampling, etc). This is so because
quantitative research is more structured and controlled in nature. Also, the scope
of this type of research is more universal in nature, and can be defined acccurately.
The points of departure here are the following (Neser et al 1995:43):
1. Natural
and social realities are observed and studied in the same way.
2.
Scientific knowledge should be factually based on things that can be observed
and measured by means of the senses.
3.
The research process should yield value-free knowledge.
In this type of research, preference
is given to the following methods and techniques (Neser et al 1995:53):
1. conceptualisation of concepts that
can be operationalised through measuring instruments
2. data-collection techniques, such
as structured questionnaires and schedules
3. data-analysis techniques, varying
from simple cross-tabulation of the data to complex analysis techniques
The
qualitative approach
Mouton and Marais (1989:157) define
``qualitative approach'' as follows: ... that approach in which the procedures
are formalised and explicated in a not so strict manner, but in which the scope
is less defined in nature and in which the researcher does his or her
investigation in a more philosophical manner (translation).
In qualitative research, the point of
departure is to study the object, namely man, within unique and meaningful
human situations or interactions. An important aspect of this type of approach
is that often it is observation that generates the investigation.
Although qualitative research is not
based on fixed and rigid procedures it nevertheless provides the researcher
with a set of strategies with which to organise the research and to collect and
to process or to interpret data. In this type of research, preference is given
to the following methods and techniques (Neser et al 1995:53±54):
1. concepts that capture the meaning
of the experience (situation), action or interaction of the research object
(man)
2. unstructured (open) questionnaires
and interviews
3. participant observation,
ethnographic studies and case studies
4. recording of life histories, use
of autobiographies and diaries
5. analysis of collected data by
means of non-quantitative frameworks and category systems
According to Borg and Gall
(1989:385±387) qualitative research has the following general characteristics:
1. This type of research involves a
wholistic investigation executed in a natural set-up. The researcher tries to
study all the elements that are present within that particular
set-up/situation. The set-up is studied as a whole in order to understand the
realities involved. For this reason, the researcher tries to understand a phenomenon
within its social, cultural and historical context.
2. Man is the primary data-collecting
instrument in this type of research. The qualitative researcher relies on man
as observer, rather than relying on measuring instruments. The qualitative
researcher can adapt to a complex situation as it develops. Differences in
values and prejudices can be taken into account. Additional data are obtained
by means of other more objective instruments, such as documents or
questionnaires.
3. The emphasis is on the use of
qualitative methods.
4. Subjects are selected in a
purposeful, rather than a random, manner. There is a purposeful selection of a
wide variety of subjects, which can then be observed by the researcher.
5. The researcher makes use of
inductive data analysis, so that unexpected results will also come to the fore.
First, the researcher collects the data, and then he or she tries to understand
the situation and make deductions.
6. A grounded theory can be developed
that is, a theory that was actually developed from the data and that is thought
to be better than predeveloped theory (as is the case in quantitative
research). Such a grounded theory reflects the data in a more accurate manner.
There are also many quantitative researchers who acknowledge the value of a
grounded theory and who then do pilot studies in order to develop theoretical
constructs that they eventually test by means of quantitative methods.
7. The design of the research
develops as the research develops. In qualitative studies, the researcher
begins with a tentative design (sometimes even without a design), and develops
the design as the research develops. In this way, the design can be adapted and
variables can be included that had not been considered before that particular
phenomenon was observed.
8. The subject plays a role in the
interpretation of the results. Qualitative researchers try to reconstruct reality
from the subject's frame of reference. In quantitative research, the object of
study or person is called the respondent, and in qualitative research he or she
is called the subject.
9. Intuitive insights are used. In
qualitative research, the emphasis is more on intuition-based knowledge (ie, on
the subject's experience of a situation).
10. The emphasis is on social
processes. Qualitative studies focus on the social processes and the meanings
attached to such social situations by the participants.
According to Marshall and Rossman
(1989:46) the following types of research can be used within the qualitative
set-up:
1. research that, because of
practical and ethical considerations, cannot be done by means of an experiment
2. research that makes in-depth inquiries
into complexities and processes
3. research the relevant variables of
which still need to be identified
4. research that tries to find out
and explore why the current policy and practice do not work
5. research about unknown phenomena
According to Mouton and Marais
(1989:165), the methods for collecting data used by quantitative and
qualitative researchers differ in the following ways:
1. One of the characteristics of
quantitative researchers is that they use a system as a point of departure for
their research. This system is then applied to the phenomenon that is
investigated: for example, they will use a structured schedule for an
interview, or response categories in a questionnaire or test. In other words, a
specific structure is imposed on the phenomenon.
In qualitative research, on the other
hand, the point of departure is that the phenomenon should be self-evident that
is, the phenomenon needs to manifest itself as it is, and the researcher will
register this (translation).
2. Quantitative researchers look at
the phenomenon from a distance a disadvantage being that they then have
problems accommodating behavioural manifestations that were not anticipated in
the research.
Qualitative researchers, on the other
hand, are more involved in and with the phenomenon. Sometimes they are even
prepared to be part of the phenomenon that is studied: for example as members
of a gang. This would give them the opportunity to describe their own
experiences, from their own observations. Qualitative researchers are therefore
more open to observation and to pinpoint behaviour or conduct accurately.
Multiple
methods or triangulation
Researchers soon came to realise that
although qualitative research and quantitative research do differ, they can
also, in specific areas, complement each other.
Researchers then realised that the
two methods could be used in conjunction, that is, on a complementary basis.
Because of the complicity in a
``confused reality'' it is difficult to study/investigate a phenomenon in its
totality. In this complex reality, multiple methods (also called ``triangulation'')
afford a partial solution (Borg & Gall 1989:393).
The concept of ``triangulation'' is
used by surveyors. When surveying a piece of land, surveyors make use of
various viewpoints or beacons from which to measure this same piece of land
from different angles. In this way, they can identify a true fixed point.
The social scientist uses the concept
of ``multiple methods'' or ``triangulation'' to denote the use of various
measuring instruments for collecting data. The data-collection techniques used
are, among others, tests, direct observation, interview(s), content analysis,
and their purpose is to be able to investigate the same variable(s) in a
specific phenomenon. According to Shipman (in Borg & Gall 1989:393) the use
of a single method or technique for a specific phenomenon is like ``... a
one-dimensional snapshot of a very wide and deep social scene''.
If various methods and techniques are
used for measuring the same variables and these measuring instruments yield
identical results, it will lead to a greater and deeper measure of belief in
these instruments. The main advantage of this type of research is that if there
were to be only one measuring instrument for measuring the same phenomenon the
investigation would be even more reliable and valid (Neuman 1997:151).
This type of investigation actually
involves repetition in investigating the same phenomenon. This is done by the
sequential and simultaneous use of quantitative and qualitative methods, thus
developing a hypothesis or hypotheses through the qualitative method, and
testing it or them through the quantitative method.
It is important, however, not to use
the two methods at random or for
convenience' sake. An example here would be a quantitative investigation in
which the respondents are given questionnaires. If there are a few ``open''
questions in the questionnaire it still does not mean that the researcher now
also makes use of the qualitative method.
Nieswiadomy (1993:160±161) points out
that the underlying meaning of the data should be investigated thoroughly and
taken into account.
Multiple methods have the following
distinctive characteristics (Neser et al 1995:59):
1. Research decisions are based on
assumptions that include both the quantitative and the qualitative approaches
to research.
2. A style of research is used that
includes both quantitative and qualitative methods and techniques.
3. The data are used in a
complementary manner.
Comparing
Qualitative approach and Quantitative approach in research
Basically,
quantitative research is objective; qualitative is subjective. Quantitative
research seeks explanatory laws; qualitative research aims at in-depth
description. Qualitative research measures what it assumes to be a static
reality in hopes of developing universal laws. Qualitative research is an
exploration of what is assumed to be a dynamic reality. It does not claim that
what is discovered in the process is universal, and thus, replicable. Common
differences usually cited between these types of research include.
In
general, qualitative research generates rich, detailed and valid (process) data
that contribute to in-depth understanding of the context. Quantitative research
generates reliable population based and generalizable data and is well suited
to establishing cause-and-effect relationships. The decision of whether to
choose a quantitative or a qualitative design is a philosophical question.
Which
methods to choose will depend on the nature of the project, the type of
information needed, the context of the study and the availability of recourses
(time, money, and human).
It
is important to keep in mind that these are two different philosophers, not
necessarily polar opposites. In fact, elements of both designs can be used
together in mixed-methods studies.
Combining
of qualitative and quantitative research is becoming more and more common.
Every
method is different line of sight directed toward the same point, observing
social and symbolic reality. The use of multiple lines of sight is called
triangulation.
It
is a combination of two types of research. It is also called pluralistic
research.
Advantages
of combining both types of research include: research development (one approach
is used to inform the other, such as using qualitative research to develop an
instrument to be used in quantitative research)
Increased
validity (confirmation of results by means of different data sources)
Complementarities
(adding information, i.e. words to numbers and vice versa)
Creating
new lines of thinking by the emergence of fresh perspectives and
contradictions.
Barriers
to integration include philosophical differences, cost, inadequate training and
publication bias.
Qualitative data analysis
Qualitative
analysis involves a continual interplay between theory and analysis. In analysing
qualitative data, we seek to discover patterns such as changes over time or
possible causal links between variables.
Qualitative
approach
Qualitative
Research is collecting, analyzing, and interpreting data by observing what
people do and say. Whereas, quantitative research refers to counts and measures
of things, qualitative research refers to the meanings, concepts, definitions,
characteristics, metaphors, symbols, and descriptions of things.
Qualitative
research is much more subjective than quantitative research and uses very
different methods of collecting information, mainly individual, in-depth
interviews and focus groups. The nature of this type of research is exploratory
and open-ended. Small numbers of people are interviewed in-depth and/or a
relatively small number of focus groups are conducted.
Participants
are asked to respond to general questions and the interviewer or group
moderator probes and explores their responses to identify and define people’s
perceptions, opinions and feelings about the topic or idea being discussed and
to determine the degree of agreement that exists in the group. The quality of
the finding from qualitative research is directly dependent upon the skills,
experience and sensitive of the interviewer or group moderator.
This
type of research is often less costly than surveys and is extremely effective
in acquiring information about people’s communications needs and their responses
to and views about specific communications.
Qualitative
approach produce detailed and non-quantitative accounts of small groups,
seeking to interpret the meanings people make of their lives in natural
settings, on the assumption that social interactions from an integrated set of
relationships best understood by inductive procedures.
Qualitative approach is especially
interested in how ordinary people observe and describe their lives’. It is an
umbrella term covering types of research. Almost all share certain features:
-The core concern is to seek out and interpret the meanings that people
bring to their own actions, rather than describing any regularities or
statistical associations between “variables”
-They treat actions as part of
holistic social process and context,
rather than as something that can be extracted and studied in isolation
-They set out to encounter social
phenomena as they naturally occur (observing what happens, rather
than making it happen)
-They operate at less abstract and generalized level of explanation
-They utilize non-representative, small samples of people, rather than working
from large representative simples to identify the broad sweep of national
patterns
-They focus on the detail of human life
-Rather than starting with a theoretical
hypothesis, and trying to test it, they explore the data they encounter and
allow ideas to emerge from them (i.e. using inductive , not deductive, logic)
In qualitative approach, there is no prior social order, or social
structure external to the lived experiences of the actors, that predetermines
outcomes. It makes little sense to seek general “laws” of how “society” works,
because society is only the sum total of the many complex social situations
that are going on at time.
Only qualitative approach, with their
detailed, flexible, sensitive and naturalistic characteristics, are suited to
producing adequate sociological accounts.
Quantitative
approach
Quantitative
methods (only using deductive logic) seek regularities inhuman lives, by separating
the social world into empirical components called variables which can be
represented numerically as frequencies or rate, whose associations with each
other can be explored by statistical techniques, and accessed through
research-introduced stimuli and
systematic measurement.
Quantitative
Research options have been predetermined and a large number of respondents are involved.
By definition, measurement must be objective, quantitative and statistically
valid.
Simply put, it’s about
numbers, objective hard data. The sample size for a survey is calculated by statisticians
using formulas to determine how large a sample size will be needed from a given
population in order to achieve findings with an acceptable degree of accuracy.
Generally, researchers seek sample sizes which yield findings with at least a
95% confidence interval (which means that if you repeat the survey 100 times,
95 times out of a hundred, you would get the same response), plus/minus a
margin error of 5 percentage points. Many surveys are designed to produce a
smaller margin of error.
Quantitative methods’ is an umbrella
term covering different types of research . In its simple form, it consists of
counting of how frequently things happen (e.g. educational qualification levels
among school leaves, attendance at doctors’ surgeries, rates of divorces,
proportion of national population living below the poverty line and the
presentation of these frequencies as summaries in tables and graphs. This can
be extended by looking at how two or more factors seem to be connected,
Almost all forms of quantitative
research share certain features:
-
The core concern is to describe and account regularities in social behaviour,
rather than seeking out and interpreting the meanings that people bring to
their own actions.
-
Patterns of behaviour can be separated out into variables, and represented by numbers
-
Explanations are expressed as associations (usually statistical) between variables, ideally in form that
enables prediction of outcomes from know regularities
-
They explore social phenomena not just they
naturally occur, but by introducing stimuli
like survey questions, collecting data by systematic, repeated and
controlled measurements.
-
They are based on assumption that social
processes exist outside of individual actors’ comprehension, constraining
individual actions, and accessible to researchers by virtue of prior
theoretical and empirical knowledge.
Quantitative approach lays greater stress on prior social order or
social structures external to the actors as contributing to the shaping of
outcomes
They often test theoretical
hypotheses (i.e.using deductive not inductive
logic)
Most quantitative research operates
with less detail than qualitative research method, but with a wider scope and
more generalized level of explanation
It utilizes representative simple to
control for variations between people. Sometime this based on programmatic
decisions.
Source: GEOFF PAYNE and JUDY PAYNE (2004, p. 175) Key Concepts in Social Research, London
ETHNOGRAPHY,
the basic field research method in anthropology.
Ethnographic Research
Definition
and Background
Although a grounded theory researcher
develops a theory from examining many individuals who share in the same
process, action, or interaction, the study participants are not likely to be
located in the same place or interacting on so frequent a basis that they
develop shared patterns of behavior, beliefs, and language. An ethnographer is
interested in examining these shared patterns, and the unit of analysis is
larger than the 20 or so individuals involved in a grounded theory study. An
ethnography focuses on an entire cultural group. Granted, sometimes this
cultural group may be small (a few teachers, a few social workers), but
typically it is large, involving many people who interact over time (teachers
in an entire school, a community social work group). Ethnography is a
qualitative design in which the researcher describes and interprets the shared
and learned patterns of values, behaviors, beliefs, and language of a
culture-sharing group (Harris, 1968).
As both a process and an outcome of
research (Agar, 1980), ethnography is a way of studying a culture-sharing group
as well as the final, written product of that research. As a process,
ethnography involves extended observations of the group, most often through
participant observation, in which the researcher is immersed in the day-to-day
lives of the people and observes and interviews the group participants.
Ethnographers study the meaning of the behavior, the language, and the
interaction among members of the culture-sharing group.
Ethnography had its beginning in the
comparative cultural anthropology conducted by early 20th-century
anthropologists, such as Boas, Malinowski, Radcliffe-Brown, and Mead. Although
these researchers initially took the natural sciences as a model for research,
they differed from those using traditional scientific approaches through the
firsthand collection of data concerning existing “primitive” cultures (Atkinson
& Hammersley, 1994).
Types of
Ethnographies
There are many forms of ethnography,
such as a confessional ethnography, life history, autoethnography, feminist
ethnography, ethnographic novels, and the visual ethnography found in
photography and video, and electronic media (Denzin, 1989a; LeCompte, Millroy,
& Preissle, 1992; Pink, 2001; Van Maanen, 1988). Two popular forms of
ethnography will be emphasized here: the realist ethnography and the critical
ethnography.
The realist ethnography is a traditional approach used by cultural
anthropologists.
Characterized by Van Maanen (1988),
it reflects a particular stance taken by the researcher toward the individuals
being studied. Realist ethnography is an objective account of the situation,
typically written in the third person point of view and reporting objectively
on the information learned from participants at a site. In this ethnographic
approach, the realist ethnographer narrates the study in a third-person
dispassionate voice and reports on what is observed or heard from participants.
The ethnographer remains in the background as an omniscient reporter of the
“facts.” The realist also reports objective data in a measured style
uncontaminated by personal bias, political goals, and judgment. The researcher
may provide mundane details of everyday life among the people studied. The
ethnographer also uses standard categories for cultural description (e.g.,
family life, communication networks, worklife, social networks, status
systems). The ethnographer produces the participants’ views through closely
edited quotations and has the final word on how the culture is to be
interpreted and presented.
For many researchers, ethnography
today employs a “critical” approach (Carspecken & Apple, 1992; Madison,
2005; Thomas, 1993) by including in the research an advocacy perspective. This
approach is in response to current society, in which the systems of power,
prestige, privilege, and authority serve to marginalize individuals who are
from different classes, races, and genders.
The critical ethnography is a type of ethnographic research in which
the authors advocate for the emancipation of groups marginalized in society
(Thomas, 1993). Critical researchers typically are politically minded individuals
who seek, through their research, to speak out against inequality and
domination (Carspecken & Apple, 1992). For example, critical ethnographers might
study schools that provide privileges to certain types of students, or
counseling practices that serve to overlook the needs of underrepresented groups.
The major components of a critical ethnography include a value-laden
orientation, empowering people by giving them more authority, challenging the
status quo, and addressing concerns about power and control. A critical ethnographer
will study issues of power, empowerment, inequality, inequity, dominance,
repression, hegemony, and victimization.
Procedures
for Conducting an Ethnography
As with all qualitative inquiry,
there is no single way to conduct the research in ethnography. Although current
writings provide more guidance to this approach than ever the approach taken
here includes elements of both realist ethnography and critical approaches. The
steps I would use to conduct ethnography are as follows:
• Determine if ethnography is the
most appropriate design to use to study the research problem. Ethnography is
appropriate if the needs are to describe how a cultural group works and to
explore the beliefs, language, behaviors, and issues such as power, resistance,
and dominance. The literature may be deficient in actually knowing how the
group works because the group is not in the mainstream, people may not be
familiar with the group, or its ways are so different that readers may not
identify with the group.
• Identify and locate a
culture-sharing group to study. Typically, this group is one that has been
together for an extended period of time, so that their shared language,
patterns of behavior, and attitudes have merged into a discernable pattern.
This may also be a group that has been marginalized by society. Because
ethnographers spend time talking with and observing this group, access may
require finding one or more individuals in the group who will allow the
researcher in—a gatekeeper or key informants (or participants).
• Select cultural themes or issues to
study about the group. This involves the analysis of the culture-sharing group.
The themes may include such topics as enculturation, socialization, learning,
cognition, domination, inequality, or child and adult development (LeCompte,
Millroy, & Preissle, 1992).
As discussed by Hammersley and
Atkinson (1995), Wolcott (1987, 1994b), and Fetterman (1998), the ethnographer
begins the study by examining people in interaction in ordinary settings and by
attempting to discern pervasive patterns such as life cycles, events, and
cultural themes. Culture is an amorphous term, not something “lying about”
(Wolcott, 1987, p. 41), but something researchers attribute to a group when
looking for patterns of their social world. It is inferred from the words and
actions of members of the group, and it is assigned to this group by the
researcher. It consists of what people do (behaviors), what they say
(language), the potential tension between what they do and ought to do, and
what they make and use, such as artifacts (Spradley, 1980).
Fetterman (1998) discusses how
ethnographers describe a holistic perspective of the group’s history, religion,
politics, economy, and environment. Within this description, cultural concepts
such as the social structure, kinship, the political structure, and the social
relations or function among members of the group may be described.
• To study cultural concepts,
determine which type of ethnography to use. Perhaps how the group works needs
to be described, or the critical ethnography may need to expose issues such as
power, hegemony, and to advocate for certain groups. A critical ethnographer,
for example, might address an inequity
in society or some part of it, use the research to advocate and call for
changes, and specify an issue to explore, such as inequality, dominance,
oppression, or empowerment.
• Gather information where the group
works and lives. This is called fieldwork (Wolcott, 1999). Gathering the types
of information typically needed in ethnography involves going to the research
site, respecting the daily lives of individuals at the site, and collecting a
wide variety of materials. Field issues of respect, reciprocity, deciding who
owns the data, and others are central to ethnography. Ethnographers bring a
sensitivity to fieldwork issues (Hammersley & Atkinson, 1995), such as
attending to how they gain access, giving back or reciprocity with the
participants, and being ethical in all aspects of the research, such as
presenting themselves and the study. LeCompte and Schensul (1999) organize
types of ethnographic data into observations, tests and measures, surveys,
interviews, content analysis, interviews, elicitation methods, audiovisual
methods, spatial mapping, and network research. From the many sources
collected, the ethnographer analyzes the data for a description of the
culture-sharing group, themes that emerge from the group, and an overall
interpretation (Wolcott, 1994b). The researcher begins by compiling a detailed
description of the culture-sharing group, focusing on a single event, on
several activities, or on the group over a prolonged period of time. The
ethnographer moves into a theme analysis of patterns or topics that signifies
how the cultural group works and lives.
• Forge a working set of rules or
patterns as the final product of this analysis. The final product is a holistic
cultural portrait of the group that incorporates the views of the participants
(emic) as well as the views of the researcher (etic). It might also advocate
for the needs of the group or suggest changes in society to address needs of
the group. As a result, the reader learns about the culture-sharing group from
both the participants and the interpretation of the researcher. Other products
may be more performance based, such as theater productions, plays, or poems.
Challenges
Ethnography is challenging to use for
the following reasons. The researcher needs to have grounding in cultural
anthropology and the meaning of a social-cultural system as well as the concepts
typically explored by ethnographers.
The time to collect data is
extensive, involving prolonged time in the field. In many ethnographies, the
narratives are written in a literary, almost storytelling approach, an approach
that may limit the audience for the work and may be challenging for authors
accustomed to traditional approaches to writing social and human science
research.
There is a possibility that the
researcher will “go native” and be
unable to complete the study or be compromised in the study. This is but one
issue in the complex array of fieldwork issues facing ethnographers who venture
into an unfamiliar cultural group or system. A sensitivity to the needs of
individual studies is especially important, and the researcher needs to acknowledge
his or her impact on the people and the places being studied.
INTRODUCTION
Ethnography has become an established
component of the repertoire of research methods. It was developed in
anthropology, but in recent decades its use has spread widely in the social
sciences and the humanities. The critical evaluation of ethnographic data is
not yet well developed. Appropriate use of this type of material therefore
requires an understanding of its special strengths and weaknesses. These derive
not only from the objectives that underlie ethnographic work, both conscious
and unconscious, but also from the cultural background of the investigators,
the various factors that condition each particular relationship between
investigator and subject, and other factors that arise from the particular time
and place of the observation. All of these factors have changed over time and
continue to evolve.
The term “ethnography” first appeared
in England in the 1830s. It was coined after a German model to characterize the
burgeoning literature in English on the manners and customs of the “races”
(ethna) of the world.
By the end of the 19th century
ethnography had become the general term for qualitative data on other cultures
and societies, and also for the field inquiries that produced them. It began as
the study of tribal, non-literate societies.
Later, and especially since the
1950s, it has moved progressively into the arena of contemporary societies
generally, including modern industrial societies, and now deals consciously
with the entirety of the human record. It developed as an integral component of
anthropology (within the branch sometimes called ethnology, later mainly social
or cultural anthropology), which was first established in major universities in
England and America in the 1880s with the mandate to document and make sense of
the full range of human variation.
The first landmark in the history of
ethnography was the compilation in 1839 (long before anthropology was
professionalized), by a committee of the British Association for the
Advancement of Science (Section H) and the Anthropological Institute in London,
of the lines of inquiry to be pursued by ethnographers, entitled “Notes and
Queries on Anthropology.” This handbook was updated in successive editions until
1971. However, ethnography was formally conceptualized as a systematic research
method by Bronislaw Malinowski (see Argonauts of the Western Pacific, London,
1922, especially pp. 2-25).
The hallmark of the ethnographic
method as developed in England after Malinowski is intensive participant
observation over an extended period in a culturally alien community.
The ethnographer gathers data on the
day-to-day community life by observing while actually participating in it. The
work usually extends over at least one full year, and is conducted in the
vernacular language.
Observation is supplemented by
discussion with local informants. The core of anthropological training for
ethnographic research consists primarily in the comparative study of
ethnographic reports from various parts of the world. This study culminates
typically in a first ethnographic experience, often lasting as much as two
years, which is intensely personal in both emotional and more general
psychological terms, and occasionally traumatic. Many of the ethnographic
monographs in the bibliography below, and most of the dissertations, derive
from such an experience. For some this was the only such experience, and if
successful (from the professional point of view) the field notes might be mined
for data to support various theoretical arguments throughout the ensuing
professional career.
Ethnographers do not observe randomly
or report comprehensively. Each ethnographic field project is designed within
the framework of the evolving corpus of theoretical discussions on the
explanation of variation and difference in human thought and behavior from
community to community. Although the development of theoretical awareness among
ethnographers was gradual, by the 1950s it had become at least equally as important
in the choice of a research community as the objective of finding new material
from areas of the world as yet unstudied. It required them, for example, to
demonstrate how local, culturally specific ways of seeing the world made sense
in their own terms, how community life “worked” in the sense that each unit of
the ethnographer’s description served to reinforce the cohesive functioning of
the life of the community. Other more complex ways of making sense of
ethnographic data, and of formulating problems for ethnographic investigation
have emerged since the middle of the century.
The primary objective of professional
ethnography is therefore both descriptive and synthetic: the extrapolation of
patterns from what can be recorded of the everyday life of a community for the
purpose of comparison and contrast with similar data from elsewhere, and the
further development of our theoretical understanding of social and cultural
processes.
The ethnographer’s cross-cultural
training enables him to record and make sense of the experience of a community
of which he has no previous experience according to an agenda developed from a
broad exposure, both personal and vicarious, to cultural variation in general,
in order to make cross-cultural comparisons that will assist in the study not
only of particular cultural processes, but also of the human condition in
general—human nature, irrespective of cultural variation.
The strengths of this orientation
inevitably bring with them a number of potential weaknesses. The need to see
order in observed phenomena by extrapolating regularities encourages the
ethnographer not only (a) to be more interested in pattern than process, but
(b) to assume that modernization and various external influences have disrupted
many regularities and (c) to reconstruct a recent past when such influences can
be assumed not to have been operating. This assumption is often encouraged by
local informants.
Awareness of the various ways in
which these strengths and weaknesses affect the ethnographic product make it
significantly more valuable. To begin with, it is important to understand how
ethnographers formulate their research problems and organize the larger
ethnographic library for which the data are gathered. They have always been
confronted by the challenge of how to reduce the infinite variety of social and
cultural data to some sort of order. They therefore classify it, and develop
their research projects in the light of their classification, which then in
turn conditions what they pay most attention to in their observations and
documentation.
Conventional ethnographic research is
most likely to produce significant results in open societies where ordinary
people readily welcome foreigners not only as transient guests but as intimate
participants in their daily lives.
However, although it is difficult to
do ethnography in conditions inimical to foreigners, the determination required
to do it in relatively closed conditions can sometimes produce rich data. In
some parts of the world (for example, much of Africa) villagers typically
welcome the opportunity to discuss community life and traditions with an
ethnographer; in others, an ethnographer’s inquisitive participation tends to
be unwelcome and is steered away from many of the obvious topics of
anthropological interest.
It is not surprising that Islamic
interest in ensuring the privacy of the household inhibits inquiries that by
their nature intrude on the privacy of family life. It may be particularly
difficult for the ethnographer to break into the public life of a community
without the advantage, at least to begin with, of quasi-socialization in the
private arena.
For these reasons although
ethnographic activity peaked in Iran in the 1970s, the corpus that has resulted
contains relatively little of the rich analytical description of social interaction
that characterizes the best work from some other parts of the world.
More recently ethnography has been
accused of the opposite fault. It has been indicted, along with “Orientalism”
(in the meaning promoted by Edward Said, in Orientalism, New York, 1978), for
political bias inspired by, and serving the purposes of, the imperialist
governments under which it evolved. According to this criticism it evinces the
writers’ sense of their own military and cultural, perhaps even natural,
superiority.
Partly because of the
anthropologist’s ahistorical interest in generalization, ethnography is often
written in an artificially timeless “ethnographic present.” A Western tendency
to see all life outside the modern West as somehow timeless makes it easy to overlook
the temporal specificity of all data. Although this problem is less conspicuous
in more recent work, because of the increased tempo of change all over the
world it is still necessary to emphasize that all ethnographic data need to be
carefully historicized before use.
The accelerating rate of social and
cultural change and the growing fragmentation of modern societies is generally
reflected in ethnographic writing, but no useful summary of how ethnography has
recently been evolving has yet appeared. Since the 1950s ethnography has
changed in significant ways, to the extent that data from different decades are
not always directly comparable. Changes in theoretical interest have changed
what ethnographers look for, and what they see and record.
Any treatment of ethnography must
take account of change, both in the culture under study and in the home
culture. The degree to which ethnography should be, or even can be, an
objective method of science or a reflexive method of the humanities and the
arts is an issue that has arisen since the 1970s and is unresolved.
Ethnography is not the only
anthropological research method. Although it remains the most characteristic
field research method, much anthropological work, especially in recent decades,
is at least partly based on one or more complementary methods, such as
ethnohistory (the correlation of documentary and various circumstantial
materials with oral history), and other methods borrowed and adapted from
linguistics, history, and the other social sciences.
It is typically constrained by
deadlines, a condition which tends to vitiate the advantages of the method, but
it should be noted that other market pressures have also conspired to reduce
the average length of all ethnographic field seasons.
Phenomenology:
Study of Lived Experience
In the literature, there continues to
be much disagreement about the meaning of phenomenology possibly because the
term has been used so widely. For instance, phenomenology has been
conceptualised as a philosophy, a research method and an overarching
perspective from which all qualitative research is sourced (Maykut & Morehouse,
1994).
If ethnography has its roots in the
discipline of anthropology, phenomenology is grounded in early-20th-century
continental philosophy, particularly of Heidegger (1962) and Husserl (1913).
The beginning and end point of
phenomenological research is lived
experience. Lived experience has a temporal structure: it can never be
grasped in its immediate manifestation but only reflectively as past presence.
The interpretive assumption of lived experience treats experience as a text;
understanding the meaning of the whole, and vice versa. Thus, a meaningful
interpretation consists of back-and-forth movement between parts and whole.
Understanding cannot be pursued in the absence of context and interpretive
framework.
In hermeneutic perspective, human experience is context-bound and
there can be no context-free or neutral scientific language with which to
express what happens in the social world. At best we could have laws applying
only to a limited context for a limited time.
Phenomenology is a human science
which studies persons. In research terminology one often uses
"subjects" to refer to the persons involved in one's study. The concept
of subject masks individual differences, blurring the uniqueness of real
people. In contrast, the word "persons" emphasizes the uniqueness of
each human being.
Unlike ethnography, phenomenology is
a philosophical rather than a methodological orientation. It is relatively
recently that the epistemological positions were translated into methods (cf.,
Thompson, 1985; van Manen, 1990). The major instruments are open ended
interviews and reflective journals. Phenomenological methods differ from
ethnographic methods in that they are not field oriented nor naturalistic:
conducting interviews and eliciting journals are, by definition, not
"natural" activities, but strategies intended to facilitate
reflection.
Types of
Phenomenology
Two approaches to phenomenology are
highlighted in this discussion: hermeneutic
phenomenology (van Manen, 1990) and empirical,
transcendental, or psychological phenomenology (Moustakas, 1994). Van Manen
(1990) is widely cited in the health literature (Morse & Field, 1995). An
educator, van Manen, has written an instructive book on hermeneutical
phenomenology in which he describes research as oriented toward lived
experience (phenomenology) and interpreting the “texts” of life (hermeneutics)
(van Manen, 1990, p. 4).
Although van Manen does not approach
phenomenology with a set of rules or methods, he discusses phenomenology
research as a dynamic interplay among six research activities. Researchers
first turn to a phenomenon, an “abiding concern” (p. 31), which seriously
interests them (e.g., reading, running, driving, mothering). In the process,
they reflect on essential themes, what constitutes the nature of this lived
experience. They write a description of the phenomenon, maintaining a strong
relation to the topic of inquiry and balancing the parts of the writing to the
whole. Phenomenology is not only a description, but it is also seen as an
interpretive process in which the researcher makes an interpretation (i.e., the
researcher “mediates” between different meanings; van Manen, 1990, p. 26) of
the meaning of the lived experiences.
Moustakas’s (1994) transcendental or
psychological phenomenology is focused less on the interpretations of the
researcher and more on a description of the experiences of participants. In
addition, Moustakas focuses on one of Husserl’s concepts, epoche (or
bracketing), in which investigators set aside their experiences, as much as
possible, to take a fresh perspective toward the phenomenon under examination.
Hence, “transcendental” means “in which everything is perceived freshly, as if
for the first time” (Moustakas, 1994, p. 34). Moustakas admits that this state
is seldom perfectly achieved.
However, I see researchers who
embrace this idea when they begin a project by describing their own experiences
with the phenomenon and bracketing out their views before proceeding with the
experiences of others.
The procedures, illustrated by
Moustakas (1994), consist of identifying a phenomenon to study, bracketing out
one’s experiences, and collecting data from several persons who have
experienced the phenomenon. The researcher then analyzes the data by reducing
the information to significant statements or quotes and combines the statements
into themes. Following that, the researcher develops a textural description of
the experiences of the persons (what participants experienced), a structural
description of their experiences (how they experienced it in terms of the
conditions, situations, or context), and a combination of the textural and
structural descriptions to convey an overall essence of the experience.
Procedures
for Conducting Phenomenological Research
I use the psychologist Moustakas’s
(1994) approach because it has systematic steps in the data analysis procedure
and guidelines for assembling the textual and structural descriptions. The
conduct of psychological phenomenology has been addressed in a number of
writings, including Dukes (1984), Tesch (1990), Giorgi (1985, 1994),
Polkinghorne (1989), and, most recently, Moustakas (1994). The major procedural
steps in the process would be as follows:
• The researcher determines if the
research problem is best examined using a phenomenological approach. The type
of problem best suited for this form of research is one in which it is
important to understand several individuals’ common or shared experiences of a
phenomenon. It would be important to understand these common experiences in
order to develop practices or policies, or to develop a deeper understanding
about the features of the phenomenon.
• A phenomenon of interest to study,
such as anger, professionalism, what it means to be underweight, or what it
means to be a wrestler, is identified. Moustakas (1994) provides numerous
examples of phenomena that have been studied.
• The researcher recognizes and
specifies the broad philosophical assumptions of phenomenology. For example,
one could write about the combination of objective reality and individual
experiences. These lived experiences are furthermore “conscious” and directed
toward an object. To fully describe how participants view the phenomenon,
researchers must bracket out, as much as possible, their own experiences.
• Data are collected from the
individuals who have experienced the phenomenon.
Often data collection in
phenomenological studies consists of indepth interviews and multiple interviews
with participants. Polkinghorne (1989) recommends that researchers interview
from 5 to 25 individuals who have all experienced the phenomenon. Other forms
of data may also be collected, such as observations, journals, art, poetry,
music, and other forms of art. Van Manen (1990) mentions taped conversations,
formally written responses, accounts of vicarious experiences of drama, films,
poetry, and novels.
• The participants are asked two
broad, general questions (Moustakas, 1994): What have you experienced in terms
of the phenomenon? What contexts or situations have typically influenced or
affected your experiences of the phenomenon? Other open-ended questions may
also be asked, but these two, especially, focus attention on gathering data
that will lead to a textural description and a structural description of the
experiences, and ultimately provide an understanding of the common experiences
of the participants.
• Phenomenological data analysis
steps are generally similar for all psychological phenomenologists who discuss
the methods (Moustakas, 1994; Polkinghorne, 1989). Building on the data from
the first and second research questions, data analysts go through the data
(e.g., interview transcriptions) and highlight “significant statements,”
sentences, or quotes that provide an understanding of how the participants
experienced the phenomenon.
Moustakas (1994) calls this step horizonalization. Next, the researcher
develops clusters of meaning from these significant statements into themes.
• These significant statements and
themes are then used to write a description of what the participants
experienced (textural description). They are also used to write a description
of the context or setting that influenced how the participants experienced the
phenomenon, called imaginative variation or structural description. Moustakas
(1994) adds a further step: Researchers also write about their own experiences
and the context and situations that have influenced their experiences.
• From the structural and textural
descriptions, the researcher then writes a composite description that presents
the “essence” of the phenomenon, called the essential, invariant structure (or
essence). Primarily this passage focuses on the common experiences of the participants.
For example, it means that all experiences have an underlying structure (grief
is the same whether the loved one is a puppy, a parakeet, or a child). It is a descriptive
passage, a long paragraph or two, and the reader should come away from the
phenomenology with the feeling, “I understand better what it is like for
someone to experience that” (Polkinghorne, 1989, p. 46).
Challenges
A phenomenology provides a deep
understanding of a phenomenon as experienced by several individuals. Knowing
some common experiences can be valuable for groups such as therapists,
teachers, health personnel, and policymakers. Phenomenology can involve a
streamlined form of data collection by including only single or multiple
interviews with participants.
Using the Moustakas (1994) approach
for analyzing the data helps provide a structured approach for novice
researchers. On the other hand, phenomenology requires at least some
understanding of the broader philosophical assumptions, and these should be
identified by the researcher.
The participants in the study need to
be carefully chosen to be individuals who have all experienced the phenomenon
in question, so that the researcher, in the end, can forge a common
understanding.
Bracketing personal experiences may
be difficult for the researcher to implement. An interpretive approach to
phenomenology would signal this as an impossibility (van Manen, 1990)—for the
researcher to become separated from the text. Perhaps we need a new definition
of epoche or bracketing, such as suspending our understandings in a reflective
move that cultivates curiosity (LeVasseur, 2003). Thus, the researcher needs to
decide how and in what way his or her personal understandings will be
introduced into the study.
HERMENEUTIC
PHENOMENOLOGY
Understanding hermeneutic
phenomenology as a research method requires the definition and discussion of
terms that may initially appear daunting – beginning with the phrase
“hermeneutic phenomenology” itself. Phenomenology is the study of experience,
particularly as it is lived and as it is structured through consciousness.
“Experience” in this context refers not so much to accumulated evidence or
knowledge as something that we “undergo.” It is something that happens to us,
and not something accumulated and mastered by us.
Phenomenology asks that we be open to
experience in this sense. Hermeneutics, for its part, is the art and science of
interpretation and thus also of meaning.
Meaning in this context is not a
thing that is final and stable, but something that is continuously open to new
insight and interpretation. Hermeneutic phenomenology is consequently the study
of experience together with its meanings. Like hermeneutics, this type of
phenomenology is open to revision and reinterpretation: it is about an openness
to meaning and to possible experiences. Hermeneutic phenomenology, in short, is
as much a disposition and attitude as it is a distinct method or program for
inquiry. As Max van Manen, one of the principle proponents of hermeneutic
phenomenology as a research method, puts it: This approach represents an
“attitude or disposition of sensitivity and openness: it is a matter of
openness to everyday, experienced meanings as opposed to theoretical ones”
(2002a, n.p.).
As it is considered in this collection,
namely as a qualitative research method in educational (and related) research,
hermeneutic phenomenology is clearly distinct from other qualitative research
methods, and also from other phenomenological approaches. It rejects the claim
of some phenomenological methods that ideal “essences” of experience or
consciousness can be isolated outside of the researcher’s cultural and
historical location. In its emphasis on the interpretation and reinterpretation
of meaning, it rejects any “transcendental” claim to meaning or any research
conclusions that are fixed once and for all. It does not study objects or phenomena
as (potentially) objective, but as necessarily meaningful. As Emmanuel Levinas
says, it does not seek to “understand the object, but its meaning” (1987, p. 110,
italics added). Also, unlike many other phenomenological and qualitative research
approaches, hermeneutic phenomenology is particularly open to literary and
poetic qualities of language, and encourages aesthetically sensitized writing
as both a process and product of research.
Phenomenology has its origins in the
work of Edmund Husserl, who framed it primarily in philosophical terms –
specifically as study of “essences,” of transcendental, ideal structures of
consciousness. Since Husserl’s time, phenomenology as both a philosophy and
method of inquiry has developed in a number of different directions, often
reflecting distinct philosophical orientations.
One of the key occurrences in this
history is its movement from the idealist or “transcendental” realm of essences
to the “immanent” world of everyday objects and concerns. This development, as
well as others in the history of hermeneutic phenomenology, is marked through
the contributions of key philosophical figures.
Some of the most celebrated are
Martin Heidegger, Maurice Merleau-Ponty,
Emmanual Levinas, and Jean-Paul
Sartre, who have both widened and deepened its philosophical features.
Heidegger, a student of Husserl, played a particularly important (and at times
problematic) role in emphasizing the phenomenology’s concern with “immanence,”
and in connecting it with hermeneutics. Heidegger articulated these emphases or
shifts in the program of phenomenology by placing priority on the study of
“being,” on how we find ourselves or simply “are” in the world. This is a type
of study otherwise known as “ontology.”
Action
Research: The Practice Orientation
If the central point of ethnography
is culture and the central point of phenomenology is lived experience, action
research is based on the close interaction between practice, theory and change.
The objects of action research-the things that action researchers study and aim
to improve, are their own educational practices, their understandings of these
practices and the institutions in which they operate. Action research involves
intervention not only as a main feature during the data collection, but as an
explicit goal of the research. The relationship between theory and practice is
neither technical nor instrumental; it is concerned with the improvement of
educational practices, understandings, and situations (Carr & Kemmis,
1986). Thus, one major difference between action research and other qualitative
approaches is its pragmatic, practiceoriented emphasis as a primary motivation
for the research.
Hult and Lennung (1980) identified
three distinct traditions for action research:
the field of community relations, the
functioning of organizations, and schooling. All areas reflected the growing
interest in the U.S. in the application of scientific methods to the study of
social and educational programs and in the study of group dynamics (Wallace,
1987). Action research was further developed by the social psychologist, Kurt Lewin,
in the form of "change experiments." In these experiments, community
workers were trained to collaborate so that they would overcome their sense of
isolation in the field and become more effective in promoting harmonious
relationships between different ethnic groups (Adelman, 1993). Here, action
research drew on the quantitative paradigm using a variety of methods including
experimental and descriptive methods.
Action research came largely
qualitative in the projects initiated and inspired by Stenhouse (1975) and
Elliott (1991) conducted in the United Kingdom from the late 1970s to the present.
Within action research there are
several distinct views about what should be critiqued and examined. Gore and
Zeichner (1991) distinguish between four varieties of reflective teaching
practice. The first is an academic version that stresses the representation and
translation of subject matter knowledge to promote student understanding. The
second is a social efficiency version that emphasizes the thoughtful application
of particular teaching strategies that have been suggested by research on teaching.
The third is a developmentalist version that prioritizes teaching that is
sensitive to students' interest, thinking and patterns of developmental growth.
The fourth is a social reconstructionist version that stresses reflection about
the social and political context of schooling and the assessment of clssroom
actions for their ability to contribute toward greater equity, social justice
and humane conditions in schooling and society.
In ethnography, the unit of analysis
is the "sub-culture," a group of people sharing behaviors, customs
and beliefs. In phenomenology, the unit of analysis is the individual or
particular phenomenon. In action research the unit is typically one classroom, occasionally
a building. In all approaches, the examination of the case holds the potential of
transferability to other instances, but never leads to generalizability in the
positivist sense. It is up to the writer to provide sufficiently thick
descriptions so the reader can decide whether the findings are applicable to
other situations.
Earlier I referred to the use of
qualitative methods (participant observations, open ended and semi-structured
interviews) as well suited to dealing with multiple realities and collaborative
projects. The uses and functions of these particular methods, however, vary from
approach to approach. Ethnographies are naturalistic, but phenomenologies typically
are not, and action research is highly interventional. Ethnographies, based on
an "I-Them" relationship, draw on in-depth, prolonged observations;
semi-structured interviews; and, often, analysis of archival materials and
documents. Phenomenological studies draw on in-depth, open-ended interviews and
sometimes reflective journals as the main instruments to capture
co-researchers' experiences. Interpretation is based exclusively on the co-researchers'
descriptions and interpretations rather than the direct observations of the
researcher. "Facts" are regarded as starting points for understanding
personal experience rather than as central data. When observations are part of
the design, they serve the role of creating a shared situation as a common
basis between researcher and co-researcher. Action research is typically based
on observations by different actors and sometimes incorporates experimental
design.
Another dimension from which we can
examine these approaches is in terms of description, interpretation and
evaluation. Ethnography is aimed toward learning in an empathic way about a
sub-culture; typically, ethnographies refrain from evaluation of the studied
culture, an attitude which is viewed as ethnocentric. Instead, they attempt to portray
and understand the studied culture on its own terms by providing thick description
and incorporating insiders' interpretations into the manuscript.
Phenomenology, too, is oriented
toward description and interpretation rather than evaluation. In action
research, however, description and interpretation serve as means to the
teachers' evaluation of their curriculum and teaching, since the primary goal
of conducting the study is toward the immediate improvement of their
instruction. It is important, however, to point out that even though
ethnographic and phenomenological studies are initially concerned with
"understanding for its own sake," when conducted in the field of
music education they are often regarded as a tool to foster pedagogy. Indeed, one
could argue that all educational research is ultimately concerned with the improvement
of teaching and learning, and that educational theories should serve educational
practice
Finally, the criteria of merit for
each of these three research orientations vary.
Ethnographic significance is derived
socially, not statistically, from discerning how ordinary people in particular
settings make sense of the experience of their everyday lives (Wolcott, 1988).
Ethnography stresses credibility, achieved by prolonged engagement in the
setting, persistent observation and triangulation. Phenomenology emphasizes the
depth of experience captured and the gaining of fresh insights, achieved by the
establishment of trusting relationships between researcher and co-researcher.
The criteria relate to the examined reality; some aspects of these realities
may be open to triangulation and shared agreement, (e.g., teachers' behavior in
a nontraditional fiddling instruction or in a junior high school); whereas, for
others (e.g., the uniquely created reality of Suzuki students) no amount of
inquiry can produce convergence on it.
The significance of action research
is tied to its ability to produce an enhanced understanding leading to an improvement
in classroom practice. This is facilitated by multiple perspectives and clarity
of description.
These different purposes shape the
nature and style of the written report. In action research the most important
audience is the researcher and the collaborators involved in the study. In
ethnography and phenomenology, it is the larger and further scholarly community.
Ethnography and phenomenology strive for empathic understanding, or “verstehen"
(von Wright, 1970), and are best served by a scholarly style. In
practiceoriented action research, specificity and clarity is key since it is
pragmatic and focused on immediate action. Here, understanding serves action
rather than being an end in itself.
Each of these approaches can address
central concerns and gaps in our knowledge of music education. Ethnography is
best suited, perhaps, for capturing implicit and explicit values and shared
beliefs within a community. Because so much of music instruction concerns
implicit values and messages, ethnographies can be a powerful tool in
articulating and communicating those values that often play an important role
in the teaching and learning of music. Phenomenology focuses on the individual
in an attempt to capture a meaningful experience and "translate" it
into a linguistic construction
Experiences of music are central to
music activities-listening, composing and performing.
Action research aims at the direct
improvement of teaching and curriculum within a particular classroom, gaining a
more critical perspective from which the teacher/researcher can reflect and
change.
The exploration of these approaches
of inquiry has not extended to most research in music education. The
understanding of the capabilities of these approaches, their intellectual
territory, the issues they address, and the contributions they can make to the theory
and practice of music instruction can help researchers and scholars expand the boundaries
of knowledge in music education research.
Notes
1. These are only three out of
various qualitative genres. Other approaches include case-studies, protocol
analysis, symbolic and interpretive interaction, critical theory, ethnomethodology,
formative research, and feminist research.
2. Including structured,
semi-structured and unstructured interviews, life-history interview,
questionnaires and sometimes even standardized tests and related measurement techniques
(Wolcott, 1988).